Saltar a Contenido

Saltar a Menú de Opciones

eNeurobiología - Revista electrónica

Universidad Veracruzana

  • Xalapa, Ver,
  • Español / Inglés

Maternal care: efficient and inefficient mothers

 

Artículo de Revisión

 

Brug B1,2, Saft P1,2, Manzo J1, Pérez-Pouchoulen M1,2, Rojas-Durán F1, Hernández ME1, Toledo-Cárdenas R1*

1Centro de Investigaciones Cerebrales, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Ver. México 2Posgrado en Neuroetología. Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Ver, México.

 

Abstract/Resumen
Introducción
Conclusiones
Agradecimientos
Referencias
Correspondencia


Resumen

El vínculo entre la madre y su descendencia es un aspecto fundamental del desarrollo del individuo. La madre es la que brinda cuidados y procura la supervivencia del infante. La calidad de dicha atención dependerá en gran medida del estado de salud física y de un ambiente propicio con mayor oportunidad de cubrir las necesidades básicas, así como un bajo nivel de estrés. Este tema ha sido estudiado desde muchas perspectivas, como la psicológica, médica y social. Recientemente se han descrito algunos procesos neurobiológicos de la maternidad, como son; el impacto de la relación madre-cría tanto en el sistema nervioso del infante como de la madre.  En la cría esta relación afectará todo su desarrollo, desde su etapa fetal, hasta la adultez. En este trabajo exploramos lo que hasta hoy se conoce en la literatura acerca de del vínculo madre-cría en el desarrollo de un individuo sano y la correspondencia de un vínculo deficiente con alteraciones en el desarrollo de la progenie o incluso la no supervivencia.

Palabras clave: Conducta Materna, Apego materno, Mamíferos, Crías.

Inicio

 

Abstract

The bond between mother and offspring is a fundamental aspect of individual development. The mother is the one who provides care and seeks infant survival. The quality of such care will depend largely on the state of physical health and an environment with greater opportunity to meet basic needs and a low stress. This topic has been studied from many perspectives, such as psychological, medical and social. Recently described some neurobiological processes motherhood as they are; the impact of mother-offspring relationship both in the nervous system of the infant and mother. In raising this relationship affect all its development from its fetal stage to adulthood. In this paper we explore what today is known in the literature about the mother-infant in the development of a healthy individual and the link between a poor relationship and alterations in the development of progeny or even no offspring survival.

Key words: Maternal behavior, Maternal attachment, Mammals, Offspring.

Inicio

 

1. Introduction: Maternal “love”

The unconditional maternal love is not just hugging or affection offered by mothers to their children, a mother's love goes beyond a demonstration of affection which can be seen all the care provided by mothers to their children, increasing their possibilities to survive, adulthood and ensure their progeny, whatever the species to which they belong.1 In a brain evolved like most vertebrate females, there are specialized structures that scientists have managed to associate the care of infants.2 This partnership has been observed through experiments ranging from behavioral models to sophisticated models to anatomical, cellular and molecular level, focused on understanding the strong bond between mothers and their offspring.3

In all species including humans, interactions between mothers and their young require integration of external stimuli and internal states, where the signals emitted by the children are detected by the mothers, so that these and perceived reacted to infants by providing the required welfare, as the signal olfactory, auditory, visual, gustatory or somatosensory.4 Such is the case of rats where stimulation is reciprocal between mother and offspring for the deployment of maternal behavior, where the interaction between the stimuli received by the signals emitted by the pups provide important benefits to be breastfed as pups, groomed, get hot, be carried to the nest or get well anogenital licking stimulation to defecate and urinate, while this behavior is also where the mother makes a profit as it recovers water and electrolytes in the urine their young5, 6 Thus stimuli emitted by pups provide them rewarding responses from the mother.

Many of the structures involved in the performance of maternal behavior, corresponding to the limbic system, which is a set of brain areas responsible for emotions. Moreover it also requires the integration of the nervous, endocrine, sensory and motor system and the overall functioning of these systems are those that determine and guide the deployment of maternal behavior.7 For example in mammals maternal care consists of a set of motor movements and sensory events that prepare mothers from the end of pregnancy, childbirth and lactation to provide maternal care to offspring, which implies heat, food, cleanliness and protection that infants receive from birth until they are independent, so that maternal care is very important for the development and survival of infants, because all these cares favor the formation of a very strong bond between mothers and newborns, to which has been called "maternal attachment" that is very important for the development of emotional state and future behavior in adulthood.

2. Maternal Behavior and brain

For many years, both humans and animals have been observed where the positive and negative effects to those infants receiving adequate or inadequate maternal care. But, how to know if a mother has proper care of their young?

During the decade of the 70's significant progress was made in the description of the various behavioral processes involved in the mother-infant bond. In these studies it was established that these behaviors involve different multisensory processes.8, 9 In the case of rodents such as olfactory, auditory signals and motor responses such as grooming behavior, protection and breastfeeding among others, are extremely important for their development.

All of these behaviors also require cognitive processes that, together with sensory and motor processes, help the attention, memory, social recognition, motivation and learning, acquire relevance in presence of bonding between mother and breeding and consequently attachment is established.7

Affective disorders appeared at an early age affecting the development of certain neural structures, for example, infants who face stressful situations by separation from the mother for long periods, suffer irreversible changes in neurobiological processes that are reflected in adulthood.10 This is seen mainly in tasks where learning, memory, or to resolve high-risk situations or stress are involved. In these studies both in humans and in animal models,11-14 it has also been reported that in early stress reaches cause depression, schizophrenia, or addiction in adulthood..15 There are also clear examples of the effects of such short-term separation. Infants born in hospitals that are separated from their mothers for various reasons, whether those born premature and should stay in bassinets or incubators or treated by the physician, their heart rate changes, and when they return to their mothers and have direct physical contact, the basal rate resets. Moreover it has also been shown that “maternal neglect” can seriously interfere with the emotional development of children making them vulnerable to future psychiatric disorders. This vulnerability is linked to the early development phase, where several brain regions related to social behavior as the prefrontal cortex (mainly anterior cingulate and orbital) are still developing which maturation lasts until the end of adolescence. So that, early experiences are modulating the functional development of this cortical area, where the neuronal organization will affect the adaptive capacity of children.16

3. Motherhood and hormones

Hormones elicit a maternal behavior, studies such of Rosenblatt and collaborators, in 1988.17 proposed that the drop of progesterone level that take place at birth may facilitate mother behavior, but maintenance of certain displays show that hormones alone are no sufficient to promote those behaviors e.g. virgin females without circulating ovarian hormones, display maternal behavior after extensive exposure to pups.18 Although, studies about hormone levels during gestation and parturition explain how levels goes through many ups and downs, and each hormonal change has an impact in observable behavior, such affiliate, aggression, and more marked on reduce sexual behavior19 (it may lead the mother to focus on offspring), this suggest that endocrine system is important for maternal displays.

Nonetheless there are very marked hormones related to maternal care, such oxytocin (OXT) which is released during childbirth, breastfeeding and copulation and is associated with feelings of pleasure, calm, formation of attachment bonds, increased sensitivity of touch, smells, gestures and words (released at central level). Moreover, in rodents and primates that inhibits the release of stress hormones (glucocorticoids), regulates the release of dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline (Na) (attention, motivation, alertness, prolactin -maternal responses-) and opioid (welfare, pleasure). To that oxytocin perform their functions must be synthesized in the nervous system and other body tissues. In the mammalian brain the hypothalamus has nerve cells that synthesize and then release from the pituitary gland into the bloodstream, in this way prepare the uterus at the time of delivery and breast to release milk. Accordingly on this, studies in rodents, OXT regulation provides both; physical and mental benefits on infants and mothers, it helps to structure a positive social interaction in further experiences of the young, in other words, the bond among mother and offspring will be a pattern to offspring to socialize with others at their adulthood. Equally important is peptide vasopressin (AVP) that is involved in self-defense and aggression, the role in mother behavior seems to modulate along with OXT,21 although it is difficult to know specifically the role of each peptide, hormone or neurotransmitter, these enclose an interaction to maintain some features in mother behavior. Additionally stress is one important factor that has an effect on biochemical mechanisms as hormone levels,22 neuronal pathways, behavioral outcomes and molecular mechanisms.23

4. Mother-child relationship: How to benefit or affect maternal care to their children?

Various positive and negative effects related to maternal care, have shown in newborns during the first hours after delivery. Studies about the response to the mother's voice demonstrated that in newborns the left hemisphere is activated, a cerebral area related to language and learning.24 In this sense, Kennel and Klaus (1998) after making a comparison between a group of human mothers who were separated from their infants and a group of mothers who were allowed to have contact with the newborn immediately after birth, showed that the latter are the ones who developed a greater sensitivity to the needs of the infant, being more patient and loving,25 so it has been suggested that during this first meeting, the newborn stimulates the sense of the mother to detect the maternally relevant signals.

 

Inicio

Figure 1.Rat mother with her pups.  Display licking and grooming behavior.

 

In most cases infant crying is the first form of communication in its earliest stage of life, this stimulus is also the medium through which the infant expresses his need for attention. Crying baby is able to modulate the behavior of the mother, preparing to act (sense of urgency to help) and causing her emotional response that does respond empathically.26, 27

In most mammals, olfactory signals profoundly influence social reactions. These signals are emitted by urine, feces, saliva and secretions of various cutaneous glands and are often deposited by deployments often stereotyped behavior. Olfactory signals deposited by an individual are perceived by others of the same specie by the main olfactory system and vomeronasal organ 28. Olfactory stimulation influences maternal behavior, because the smell comes from any infant becomes a nice encouragement for new mothers, generating statements of attraction and alert, 29 especially during the early postpartum (1-4 days postpartum) compared to other odors from adult bodies.30 On the other hand the neonate provides vital information to the mother during the sensitive period to allow maintenance of maternal responsiveness. So deprivation of licking and suckling do not disturb maternal acceptance, the rate of rejection increased as the olfactory contact between mother and offspring decreased, for example, in one group of ewes that could not smell their lambs, the rate of acceptance was as low as in the group of mothers totally deprived of their young for 12 hr.

In rats, isolated females at birth, when reach adulthood show difficulties and shortages to display maternal behavior. So attention deficit and hyperactive be more impulsive, more emotionally reactive, compared with females reared by the mother. A similar experiment with mice, animals turned in to hyperactive and anxious behaviors, compared with another group of mice that were not separated from their mothers.31 Moreover it has been shown that the greater amount of attention, more ability to develop adequate response to stress. This was observed in rats where epigenetic studies showed that certain genes are modified depending on the amount of maternal care when they were pups and that these changes are maintained long term and can be passed from generation.32 However, research confirms that genetic imprinting in human infants is not as deterministic as in rats, but can be altered by life experiences after birth and neglect in early parenting.33

These studies helps to understand how early experiences can affect the development of offspring in infancy and in adulthood. In this sense we have a clue since maternal care and attention have an impact on the chemistry of the limbic system; consequently, maternal behavior can influence how offspring will face their entire lives.

For humans the mother-child bond affects the brain development of children. Investigations revealed that the absence or weakness of the mother-child bond in the first months of life of the young is related to anxiety, depression, aggression and hostility of the newborn, at other times in their lives. This is because the mother-child bond affects the hypothalamic -pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA axis), a system that controls reactions to stress, including segregation of a stress hormone cortisol, 34 e.g. studies in humans and animal models have confirmed that addiction is a state of security, anxiety or fear that occurs in both children and the young, which can be determined by the accessibility and responsiveness given by the mother or who is establishing the link. So the attachment is an emotional bond that develops emotional security where the young notice maternal protection.

5. Discussion: Is there a bad mother behavior?

Recently has been proposed that the age of motherhood in humans could be one factor to consider about child care, this may be due lack of parenting skills in young mothers4 and social level and genetic factors,35 these studies show that is possibly to argue that there is a “good or bad” mother behavior, although in animals there are examples of abnormal mother response in conditions such as cocaine exposure during lactancy period.36-38 We must consider the biological functions of those outcomes, and determine if there is a successful strategy in benefit to the litter, e.g. when a rat pup born with congenital malformations or abnormalities, the mother kill those pups38 infanticide has been associated with mother experience; mothers with no previous experience are more prone to kill their offspring than experienced females, although capybara females do not kill their pups due to experience, equally primiparous and multiparous show similar rate of frequency of this behavior..39 Another condition that leads to infanticide or cannibalism is food intake e.g. female hamsters fed with light diet, cannibalized more and maintain smaller litter size than females with fat diet, so it is possible that this behavior is modulated by availability of metabolic fuels.40

Inicio

6. Conclusion

Maternal care, increases the chances of survival for children and the benefits to adulthood, so ensures that the offspring. However the interaction between mother and young requires the integration of external stimuli and internal states of both parties to provide a required response. Hormones induce maternal behavior, but are not absolute for eliciting care behaviors. In mother-young bond, central neuronal Oxytocin projections play a role to establish a positive contact, and helps to establish social interactions in adulthood. In this sense, maternal neglect can seriously interfere with the emotional development of the offspring, if the mother-infant bond is absent or weak, the development is affected. Some of the failures in development of young like isolation derive in future abnormal maternal behavior when infants reach adulthood, and become more hyperactive and anxious; also in humans causes attention deficit, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Failures of mother care at early age are involved in the development of depression, schizophrenia and addiction. The state of insecurity, anxiety and fear can be determined by the accessibility and responsiveness given by the mother, or whoever is setting the emotional bond.

Inicio

 

7. Acknowledgments

Centro de Investigaciones Cerebrales Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Mexico CONACyT scholarship 240116 (BBA) 235940 (PSL) Promep Grant 103.5/07/2753 (RT)..

Inicio

 

8. References

  1. Hahn-Holbrook J, Holbrook C, Haselton MG. Parental precaution: Neurobiological means and adaptive ends. Neurosci Biobehav R 2011 35:4 1052-1066.
  2. Poindron P, Lévy F, Krehbiel D. Genital, olfactory, and endocrine interactions in the development of maternal behaviour in the parturient ewe. Psychoneuroendocrino 1988 13:1 99-125.
  3. Kristal MB. The biopsychology of maternal behavior in nonhuman mammals. Ilar J 2009 50:1 51-63.
  4. Stern DN. The first relationship: Infant and mother. Harvard University Press Cambridge, Massachusetts 2009 pp 49-68.
  5. Moore CL and Morelli GA. Mother rats interact differently with male and female offspring. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1979 93:4 677- 684.
  6. Gubernick DJ and Alberts JR. Maternal licking of young: Resource exchange and proximate controls. Physiol & Behav 1983 31:5 593-601.
  7. Díaz-Rossello JL and Ferreira-Castro A. Maternology: When a Baby is Born, a Mother is Born. Neoreviews 2008 9:8 326-331.
  8. Stenzel D and Huttner WB. Role of maternal thyroid hormones in the developing neocortex and during human evolution. Front Neuroanat 2013 16:1 7-19.
  9. Strathearn L, Fonagy P, Amico J, Montague PR. Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacol 2009 34:13 2655-2666.
  10. Hulshof HJ, Novati A, Sgoifo A, Luiten PG, den Boer JA, Meerlo P. Maternal separation decreases adult hippocampal cell proliferation and impairs cognitive performance but has little effect on stress sensitivity and anxiety in adult Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res 2011 216:2 552-560
  11. Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK. Neonatal maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Res 2002 950:1 52-63.
  12. Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Rio J, Ramirez MJ. Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience 2008 154:4 1218-1226.
  13. Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Río J, Ramírez MJ. Cognitive impairment associated to HPA axis hyperactivity after maternal separation in rats. Psychoneuroendocrino 2007 32:3 256-266.
  14. Hrdy SB. Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol Sociobiol 1979 1:1 13-40.
  15. Niwa M, Matsumoto Y, Mouri A, Ozaki N, Nabeshima T. Vulnerability in early life to changes in the rearing environment plays a crucial role in the aetiopathology of psychiatric disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011 14:04 459-477.
  16. Strathearn L. Maternal neglect: oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. J Neuroendocrinol 2011 23:11 1054-1065.
  17. Rosenblatt JS, Mayer AD, Giordano AL. Hormonal basis during pregnancy for the onset of maternal behavior in the rat. Psychoneuroendocrinol 1988 13:1 29-46.
  18. Fleming AS, Korsmit M, Deller M. Rat pups are potent reinforcers to the maternal animal: effects of experience, parity, hormones, and dopamine function. Psycho-biology 1994 22:1 44-53.
  19. Maestripieri D and Carroll KA. Behavioral and environmental correlates of infant abuse in group-living pigtail macaques. Infant Behav Dev 1998 21:4 603-612.
  20. Insel TR and Young LJ. The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2001 2:2 129-136.
  21. Bosch OJ and Neumann ID. Both oxytocin and vasopressin are mediators of maternal care and aggression in rodents: from central release to sites of action. Horm Behav 2012 61:3 293-303.
  22. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 1997 277:5332 1659-1662.
  23. Fumagalli F, Molteni R, Racagni G, Riva MA. Stress during development: Impact on neuroplasticity and relevance to psychopathology. Prog Neurobiol 2007 81:4 197-217.
  24. Beauchemin M, González-Frankenberger B, Tremblay J, Vannasing P, Martínez-Montes E, Belin P, Lassonde M. Mother and stranger: an electrophysiological study of voice processing in newborns. Cereb Cortex 2011 21:8 1705-1711.
  25. Maldonado M, Lecanneler F, Lartegue T. Aspectos evolutivos de la relación madre-bebé. Perinatol Reprod Hum 2008 22:1 15-25.
  26. Lorberbaum JP, Newman JD, Horwitz AR, Dubno JR, Lydiard RB, Hamner MB, George MS. A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biol Psychiat 2002 51:6 431-445.
  27. Stallings J, Fleming AS, Corter C, Worthman C, Steiner M. The effects of infant cries and odors on sympathy, cortisol, and autonomic responses in new mothers and nonpostpartum women. Parenting 2001 1:2 71-100.
  28. Castañeda MD, Martínez-Gómez M, Guevara-Guzmán R, Hudson R. Comunicación química en mamíferos domésticos. Chemical communication in domestic mammals. Vet. Méx, 2007 38,1.
  29. Stallings J, Fleming AS, Corter C, Worthman C, Steiner M. The effects of infant cries and odors on sympathy, cortisol, and autonomic responses in new mothers and nonpostpartum women. Parenting 2001 1:1 71-100.
  30. Fleming AS, Korsmit M, Deller M. Rat pups are potent reinforcers to the maternal animal: effects of experience, parity, hormones, and dopamine function. Psychobiology 1994 22:1 44-53.
  31. Mccormick CM, Kehoe P, Kovacs S. Corticosterone release in response to repeated, short episodes of neonatal isolation: evidence of sensitization. Int J Dev Neurosci 1998 16:3 175-185.
  32. Gudsnuk K, Champagne F. Epigenetic effects of early developmental experiences. Clin Perinatol 2011 38:703-717.
  33. Champagne FA. Epigenetic influence of social experiences across the lifespan. Dev Psychobiol 2010 52:4 299-311.
  34. Strathearn L, Fonagy P, Amico J, Montague PR. Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacol 2009 34:13 2655-2666.
  35. Feinberg M, Neiderhiser J, Howe G, Hetherington EM. Adolescent, parent, and observer perceptions of parenting: Genetic and environmental influences on shared and distinct perceptions. Child Dev 2001 72:4 1266-1284.
  36. Johns JM, Nelson CJ, Meter KE, Lubin DA, Couch CD, Ayers A, Walker CH. Dose-dependent effects of multiple acute cocaine injections on maternal behavior and aggression in Sprague-Dawley rats. Dev Neurosci 1998 20:6 5 25-532.
  37. Kinsley CH, Turco D, Bauer A, Beverly M, Wellman J, Graham AL. Cocaine alters the onset and maintenance of maternal behavior in lactating rats. Pharmacol Biochem Be 1994 47:4 857-864.
  38. Hrdy SB. Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol Sociobiol 1979 1:1 13-40.
  39. Nogueira SS, Nogueira-Filho SL, Otta E, Dias CT, de Carvalho A. Determination of the causes of infanticide in capybara Hydrochaeris hydrochaeris groups in captivity. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1999 62:4 351-357.
  40. Schneider JE and Wade GN. Effects of maternal diet, body weight and body composition on infanticide in Syrian hamsters. Physiol & Behav 1989 46:5 815-821.
Recibido: 15 de octubre de 2014 Aceptado: 25 de febrero de 2015

 

Inicio

Correspondencia:


Centro de Investigaciones Cerebrales, Universidad Veracruzana, Av. Luis Castelazo Ayala, Col. Industrial Animas, Xalapa, Ver. C.P. 91190 email: garanda@uv.mx

Este es un artículo de libre acceso distribuido bajo los términos de la licencia de Creative Commons, (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0), que permite el uso no comercial, distribución y reproducción en algún medio, siempre que la obra original sea debidamente citada.

Inicio

 

Regresar

Volúmenes